Skip to main content Skip to main navigation menu Skip to site footer
Articles
Published: 2021-12-31

Are you there? Presence in collaborative distance work

1Digital Transformation and Lifelong Learning Research Group, School of Technologies, University of Tallinn
Department of Education, Faculty of Educational Sciences, University of Helsinki
Learning Environments Research Group, Aalto University
Department of Education, Faculty of Educational Sciences, University of Helsinki
Department of Education, Faculty of Educational Sciences, University of Helsinki
Department of Education, Faculty of Educational Sciences, University of Helsinki
Department of Education, Faculty of Educational Sciences, University of Helsinki
Research-based design tracking presence computer-mediated communication collaboration at work distance work

Abstract

Already before the pandemic, digitally mediated collaborative work and communication were perceived as challenging. We investigate the attitudes towards emerging technologies and for transforming practises in workplaces. The focus lies on understanding the readiness for appropriating emotional tracking on presence and support for collaboration. The research-based design framework allowed to combine the various perspectives of the transdisciplinary team. Methods included participatory design, design thinking, contextual inquiry and prototype testing for enhancing presence while working with shared objects in video conferencing to explore the appropriation of tools. The findings revealed four indications: 1) awareness of interlocutors’ presence during synchronous communication is crucial. 2)  Emotion and behaviour tracking raises concerns about privacy and personal control over what is displayed to others, and technology could be simpler non-distracting the work at hand.  3) The prototype was found to enhance the feeling of presence without disturbing work at hand, and 4) appropriation requires a step-by-step approach.

Metrics

Metrics Loading ...

Introduction

Communication and collaboration through digital technologies are necessary aspects of almost all types of work. Not surprisingly, communication through digital technologies is still perceived to be unsatisfactory. For instance, it takes too much effort to make sense if the others are attentive and present, especially when the participants do not know each other well enough. Remote communication hinders collaborative work as participation in the work is not displayed evenly to all. (Hannola, Richter, Richter, & Stocker, 2018). The solutions thus far have not adequately solved the challenge of presence in a way that maintains concentration on the work.

Prominent perspectives into the subject can be grouped into a technology-dominant angle studying features which enable or support, for instance, participants’ feeling of other participants’ presence and practice, and an experience-dominant angle investigating requirements of how to support collaboration and communication during work. Various research areas, for instance, Human-Computer Interaction (HCI), Computer-Mediated Communication (CMC), Computer Supported Cooperative Work (CSCW), and cognitive neuroscience, have explored the challenges associated with digital communication and collaboration tools.

In this article, we approach the issues of presence, awareness, and support for collaboration from a transdisciplinary perspective. Our team consists of design researchers with expertise in co-design and design thinking, technology-enhanced work and learning research, and researchers in the field of cognitive neuroscience. Thus, the framework we use is broad, and it includes the fields of HCI, CMC, CSCW and cognitive neuroscience. CSCW has discussed various aspects of collaboration, from Grudin’s (1994) group work and social dynamics to Stahl’s (2016) recent studies on intersubjectivity and group cognition. CMC, similarly, has been digging into issues on digital, remote, and synchronous communication from Boon and Holmes (1991) discussing the dynamics of interpersonal trust to Biocca, Harms, & Burgoon (2003) on theory and measurement of social presence. HCI began with awareness features (Gutwin & Greenberg, 1998) and advanced into complex presence measures (Froese, Iizuka, & Ikegami, 2014), borrowing ideas from cognitive neuroscience. We also have first-hand experience designing some of the first awareness features into computer-supported learning and working in various international projects (e.g., Netpro1). However, digital tools have improved, and research on how presence is observed and experienced, including topics such as the relationship between inter-brain synchronisation (Balters, Mayseless, Hawthorne, & Reiss, 2021), has increased recently. We complement the new neuroscientific findings by looking back and including research-based design studies to understand the current needs and attitudes in workplaces. It seems that we are in a paradigm shift on what learning and working means. This disruptive transformation is partially initiated by digitalisation – using emerging tools such as algorithmic systems that are proposed to be implemented in all parts of life. The pandemic has been accelerating this tendency (Trenerry al., 2021).

Our objectives for the research were to understand if there is a change in the hindrances of collaboration and communication in remote work due to the forceful introduction of emerged technologies and digitalisation. The workplace sectors were based on the Business Finland project HUMEX Quantifying Human Experience for Increased Intelligence Within Work Teams and in the Customer Interface. Most sectors can be categorised as knowledge work, e.g., legal, insurance, development, design and marketing companies/customer services, and the construction sector at the decision-making level.

We used research-based design methods to investigate the current workplace hindrances and benefits of emerging technologies. The research-based design allows us to combine the different disciplines and real context on workplaces providing a thicker description of the current change, attitudes towards the new technologies and potential transformation of work cultures.

Our research is based on the hypothesis H1: there is a need to use algorithmic systems on generating emotional and behaviouraldata for tools that enhance, support and scaffold the digitally mediated collaborative work

During the research process, it became clear we had to narrow our hypothesis into (H2): Simplify the tools used for collaboration in the work setting

a) the feedback of the presence of other interlocutors must be as simplified as possible so that it can be perceived at a glance or with peripheral vision and

b) the workers must feel that they are in control of what is presented to the other interlocutors.

Next, we go through the previous research on the framework of HCI, CMC, CSCW and contrast these with current findings on cognitive neuroscience. We start with older research to build the background of what is known and could be used for new approaches. After which, we describe the methods, data collection and analysis. From there, we move to the findings and results.

Background on communication and collaboration through digital technologies

This section discusses research that has allowed us to tackle new difficulties of appropriating new tools and practises for digitally mediated communication and collaboration. 

The crucial features of digitally mediated communication are visibility of interlocutors’ actions to guide interactions and seeing that others are present and available for communication, which affects communicative behaviours and accountability. Previous research has focused on improving turn-taking (Anderson, Beard, & Walther, 2010), enhancing situation-, work-, social- and self-awareness (Dourish & Bellotti, 1992; Gross, 2013) and identifying relevant non-verbal communication cues (Cameron & Webster, 2005; Kruger, Epley, Parker, & Ng, 2005) or on evoking affective interdependencies (Salminen et al., 2018). Part of the awareness is to understand that and others are receptive to communication (Gutwin & Greenberg, 1998). Quan-Haase, Cothrel, and Wellman (2005) investigated online communication, and they explained the underlying needs for successful, digitally mediated communication and collaboration. The four needs are a) The interlocutors’ task interdependencies, their history of collaboration and informal socialising support the success of communication during collaborative work; b) Visibility results in awareness when the interlocutors know each other or share experiences; c) Increasing social cues works well when the interlocutors are acquainted with each other and have established trust; and d) The roles the interlocutors possess make a difference: lower-status employees feel a need to answer timely, and higher status employees expect responses within a short time.

Recently, the focus has been on future technologies in HCI. Mueller, Maes, and Grudin (2019) referred to as Human-Computer Integration (HInt). The HInt is divided into three classes on their ability to provoke different types of awareness in humans: on-, off- and in-body. Off-body is technology situated in the environment around the body and is not physically attached to the body. The on-body is technology existing on the surface of the body (such as wearables or hand-held devices), and the in-body is a technology that exists internally within the body (such as ingestible devices) (Mueller, Maes, & Grudin, 2019). These technologies are seen to provide to their “wearers” various benefits; for instance, off-body provides the familiar sensor information for checking the weather, location, and speed. These are often used in recreational and work practises alike. The on-body technology may be most known by health applications such as monitoring sleep patterns, heart rate and blood sugar levels. Lastly, in-body technology is often unthought-of but well-known since it includes pacemakers, hearing aids, eyesight improvements, and new technology such as memory aids and stress management. All of these known so far aim to help, support and make life easier by knowing oneself and one’s behaviour, namely one’s habits. However, the arousal information is not often used for enhancing empathy or collaboration or for interpersonal communication, but monitoring stress levels, for being calm, strong, social and smart, for instance, with mental health smartphone apps (Bakker, Kazantzis, Rickwood, & Rickard, 2016; Peake, Kerr, & Sullivan, 2018).

Although many communication tools provide rich media such as emojis, audio and video notes, and video conference features, people still need to adapt their communication and work styles to the possibilities and limitations of what the current technology supports (Koskinen, Zimmerman, Binder, Redstrom, & Wensveen, 2013; Zhao, Lampe, & Ellison, 2016). As a result, achieving successful collaboration in workplace settings requires participants’ attention and effort to overcome technological limitations. According to well-known works of Trigg and Bødker (1994) and Schmidt and Bannon (1992), achieving better tools requires that the tools evolve with use, meaning the tools support appropriation, customisation and tailoring culture as well as provide affordances for adaptation. In this article, the interest lies in determining how attitudes towards computer-mediated interactions have changed for synchronous communication and collaboration in a work context and whether this knowledge can be used to co-design tools that evolve along with the use. It is relevant to ascertain how automated tracking of interlocutors’ behaviours and emotions can impact workers’ perceived use of computer-mediated communication tools. For instance, Dourish (2003) has aptly stated that tools change practises, and modified practises change what is expected from the tools, meaning that the tools and systems should evolve with use while being situated in the intersection of technical design and social practice. Therefore, the mismatch between the flexible, mutable and evolving practises contrary to the predefined, rigid, resistant tools creates a gap between the practises and technological possibilities, called the ‘socio-technical gap’(Ackerman, 2000; Törpel, Pipek, & Rittenbruch, 2003). Wulf et al., (2015) underline that appropriation is a complex process, it takes long, and the practises undergo changes during the process. The complexity of the appropriation process is due to the many different societal levels where the newly designed artefacts connect to the known, and unknown social practises. Social practises are not abstract but embodied and routinised, which means the practises are intertwined with the environment and form habits (Wulf et al., 2015). Ramstead, Veissière, and Kirmayer (2016) described how normative practises built on the immersive participation of the agent in a social context are seen to regulate joint attention and shared intentionality. Knowing the habits of action and context will aid in supporting appropriation because it allows us to design tools and scaffolding for changing the habits (Fiore & Wiltshire, 2016).

Due to the differences between what technology can do and how people act and develop practises, it is essential to investigate how people anticipate the future, what they expect from technology and what they feel that they are willing to appropriate to transform existing digitally mediated collaborative practises at work. 

Considering the advances in emotion tracking technologies and the positive impact that these technologies are expected to bring, in this study, we explore the way potential users feel about emotion tracking tools in workplace contexts and what is expected from these technologies. One of these technologies to consider is the ones measuring arousal. Arousal tracking is in focus because it is increasingly integrated into the tools offered to workers. Arousal refers to a physiological state that denotes energy and activation, associated with being awake, alert and ready to respond. In the dimensional models of emotions, high levels of arousal are related to emotions such as surprise, while emotions such as sadness are associated with low arousal levels (Bhattacharjee et al., 2018).

Sensor-based technologies monitor arousal by collecting data on the changes in heart rate, blood pressure or skin conductance to estimate the level of alertness of a person’s physiological states (Gravina, Parastoo, Ghasemzadeh, & Fortino, 2017). These are increasingly used in natural situations, for instance, to measure heart rate variability changes using T-shirt integrated sensors and visualisations of heartbeat (Wikström et al., 2021). 

From previous research, one of the focus areas that we listed as potential is awareness, also called presence. The terms awareness and presence are sometimes used interchangeably. Awareness is divided into actions about the others involved, context, and content. Other areas emphasise the feelings or emotions, showing empathy, agreement, understanding and support for habit-forming or changing practises. The above areas can be tackled with various technologies, e.g., sensors and algorithmic systems, that have different intrusion aspects that previous and current technologies use. The novel technologies are thought to support work, and work practises (for instance, knowledge work and understanding the feelings of co-workers or clients). As the work sectors available for the research have various aims and practises, our first task was to narrow the scope by understanding the current situation and the envisioned future.

We present a case study that follows the research-based design approach to identify the main challenges that digitally mediated interactions have on synchronous communication and collaboration in work contexts. The following section will describe the process, methods, and analysis we used.

Research-based design: contextual inquiry, prototyping, data and analysis

The study has three parts: part A contextual inquiry and getting acquainted with the previous studies for creating the design hypothesis (see Figure 1, empirical-study part); and part B, where prototypes are designed and implemented for testing the hypothesis (Figure 1, prototyping). We have also outlined a beginning of the future study part C – appropriation, where the actual use at workplaces is studied longitudinally. All three parts have many iterations of data collection and analysis.

H1: Initial research hypothesis was that there is a need to use algorithmic systems on generating emotional and behavioural data for tools that enhance, support and scaffold the digitally mediated collaborative work. Algorithmic systems can provide recommendations, suggestions and sometimes interpretations based on various data collections.

H2: After part A, we had to simplify our hypothesis to be: Simplify the tools used for

collaboration in the work setting

H2 was divided into two parts:

a) the feedback of the presence of other interlocutors must be as simplified as possible so that it can be perceived at a glance or with peripheral vision and

b) the workers must feel that they are in control of what is presented to the other interlocutors.

Study part A explores whether and how tracking interlocutors’ physiological affective and arousal states could improve computer-mediated collaboration and communication and what types of attitudes people have towards the new technologies. During part B, we developed potential prototypes with the participants based on the findings. At the beginning of part C, the design prototypes were tested with the participants from the companies. Drawing on the prototype testing, we propose the design implications that promote design decisions and chosen methods.

Figure. 1. The figure presents the whole process of the design from contextual inquiry (part A), design, prototyping (part B) and a short study on appropriation, (part C) the methods used and the related outcomes.

We followed the constructive design approach (Koskinen et al. 2013) based on prototyping, frequent iterations, and encouragement for developing the prototypes (Mackay, 2000). The prototypes were developed iteratively involving representatives in all stages of the design process (Mattelmäki, 2006) for ensuring that digitally mediated communication and collaboration systems meet the participants’ needs and researchers’ aims. In particular, we adopted research-based design, which is influenced by co-design (Sanders, 2003) and the user-centred design tradition (Stewart & Shamdasani, 2014). It is critical to involve artefacts called probes during the design process (Pirinen, 2016). Facilitators provide probes in focus groups and workshops to the participants, thus allowing them to express their expectations of digitally mediated collaboration challenges with tangible objects. Using artefacts early in the design process allows participants to try out and suggest potential usage practises. This practice enables us to detect the direction in which tools might need to evolve during actual usage. The process supports using empirically grounded conceptualisations (Wulf et al., 2015).

We selected the participants to represent different economic sectors such as software development, construction, marketing, insurance and advertising, and digital legal services. The companies’ role was to provide a research context. The companies were future-oriented, executing knowledge work and using various digital tools. The selection of the companies’ participants aimed to gather as wide as possible representative worker base covering various tasks from service, design, management, and execution. The selection was inspired by including extreme participants following the design thinking methodology. We sent messages or discussed with the company representatives to acquire the best variation between workers in each company). In total, we had 30 participants (16 female and 14 males, see Table 1).

Sector Female/male Novice–expert
Design/marketing 7 females/ 3 males full range
Development 3 females /5 males full range
Insurance/customer service 4 females/ 4 males full range
Legal services 1 female/ 1 male leader/ expert
Construction 1 female/1 male middle management / novice
Total 30
Table. 1. All participants in the contextual inquiry, prototyping and appropriation.

Part a) contextual and empirical research

During the design process of part: A) contextual and empirical research, we used various methods and techniques for prompting conversations about digitally mediated work challenges and attitudes towards emotional tracking in the work context. We carefully chose all the methods and techniques to provide in-depth and thick description knowledge from the field. As mentioned, the process was cyclic with data collection and analysis. The chosen techniques were observations, focus groups, workshops, and theme interviews to ensure that attitudes and practises could be made tangible.

In this phase, we facilitated two focus group sessions, four workshops, two interviews and background benchmarking were used to collect data. The data included video data, notes and design artefacts (see Table 2, and for the whole process, Figure 1). We investigated the attitudes towards emotion tracking, how the participants see it could enhance their work, current practises and what is not working at work when it comes to digitally mediated collaboration and communication. The focus groups and workshops were formed from different sectors. The focus groups allowed us to find overlaps in the work and attitudes. These were the guiding themes that directed the design of workshops and interviews.

The outputs of focus groups were then validated in workshops using inspiration cards and journey maps. The cards provoked discussion on tools in use, collaboration in distance work, customer service challenges, communication issues, practises that work and do no work. We designed six cards in total (see Figure 3 for examples). Journey maps were on 1) Community building through instant messaging; 2) Showing engagement (presence) in an online focus group using video-conferencing (audio, no video); 3) Supporting shared understanding among groups using online video-conferencing (audio, no video construction field work2 (see Figure 3 for example). These kinds of reused outcomes are called tangible outcomes and provide better iteration of the research process and design). In this part A, the participants came from insurance and legal service/company, development, design, marketing, and construction. Female and male division overall in part A. was 13 males and 14 females. The count does not add to 30 (see Table 1, where all participants of part A B C are counted) nor to the beyond 30 in Table 2 as some participants took part in more than one focus group or workshop. The number of participants is not enough to make broad generalisations but indicative suggestions for similar types of knowledge and service types of works. Through the methods and outputs, we answer to the

H1, there is a need to use algorithmic systems on generated emotional and behavioural tools to enhance, support and scaffold the digitally mediated collaborative work.

Method Participants Length Data collection Techniques & materials used Analysis methods
Focus groups (2) Design/marketing (2), development (1) & construction (2) & insurance (1); legal company (1) people (7 on each workshop) 2 h Notes, video & audio recording, outcomes of the activities Synchronising practises (synchronous hand clapping), sketching and games Qualitative analysis using design thinking techniques, such as canvases, templates and journey maps, to organise the data & artefacts. Spatial and physical organisation jointly by researchers belong to design thinking analysis methods
Workshops (4) Design/marketing (1-2), development (1) & insurance (0-1); legal company (1) people (3-4 per each focus group) 2 h Notes & video recording Inspiration cards & scenarios
Interviews (2) Development (1) & insurance (1) company 1 h each Notes & recording Theme interviews
Table. 2. Table 2 presents the methods, participants, data collection and analysis methods during the contextual inquiry (part A). The participant number does not add to 30 because some participants attended more than once to provide continuation.

Part b) prototyping and part c) appropriating

Part B) prototyping and part C) appropriating included two different types of prototype testing. The setting of the prototype testing included two separate rooms where the workers were working on a joint document (shared artefact). Communication occurred through Skype or a similar audio/video conferencing tool. A randomly selected worker and student participated in the first two prototype testing prototypes. Both were well-acquainted with video conferences and collaboration with co-workers/learners mediated by digital tools. One researcher was in one room in the first two prototype testing, and the worker/student was in another room. The participants could not see or hear each other directly. The document, or scenario, was described to the participant. The scenario described presented the challenges with a Skype discussion and collaborative work – named: Facilitating a focus group using video-conferencing (see Appendix 1). Overall, we had three scenarios to choose from (Facilitating a focus group using video-conferencing, Group discussion using audio-conference and Community building using instant messaging). Participants told which was closest to them). Both rooms and the screens of the researcher and the worker were video-recorded. One of the researchers observed the worker, taking notes and helping if the prototype failed to function correctly. These prototype testing aimed to determine whether The Nodders prototype functioned as anticipated and how the participants felt about it.

The first two prototype testing lasted an hour, and data was collected by observations, interviews and audio recording. These were transcribed and analysed by three researchers. The second two prototype testing were executed with real workers executing actual tasks. The workers came from digital legal services and marketing companies. The selection was based on the availability of a real collaborative online task. These lasted for one hour and a half. The workers were connected through their internal tool in different rooms and were working with an actual work document. The legal services workers were used to work on a joint document from distance offices. The collaborative tool for the shared document was the company’s internal tool; however, in this setting, only half of the screen — the part where The Nodders was displayed — could be recorded because the document under discussion was classified. The second prototype testing with the marketing company followed the same setting, except the location was in the marketing company building. In both prototype testing, theme interviews, which were recorded, and during which notes were taken, were conducted afterwards. The themes of the interviews were the same as those of the first prototype testing.

The interviews were semi-structured, themed interviews. The themes were: the workers’ feelings when using the Nodders, positive and negative feedback related to the concept of the prototype and its feasibility in real work and concerns and hopes of technology and improvements for the prototype. Three researchers analysed the data by comparing the lists of relevant comments from participants, videos, and notes. Important notes were highlighted and discussed to answer the hypothesis and considered for future development. The results cannot be generalised broadly but can indicate design directions for a similar collaborative work setting. The hypotheses were: a) the feedback of the presence of other interlocutors must be as simplified as possible so that it can be perceived at a glance or with peripheral vision, and b) the workers must feel that they are in control of what is presented to the other interlocutors.

The analysis methods of the part a) contextual and empirical research

The researchers from two universities analysed part A) contextual and empirical research in a design thinking manner using different canvases, templates and three journey maps (Biocca et al., 2003) to arrange the data in various ways and study them from multiple perspectives. We transcribed the focus groups, workshops, videos, theme interview audios and observation notes. These were then sorted out into topics emerging from the data. The classification of relevant topics took three iterations. The researchers focused on physical and spatial arrangements of data to categorise them bottom-up. Spatial manner is supported in design thinking, providing tangible visual outcomes (for instance, the earlier mentioned topic descriptions canvases, inspiration cards, journey maps, scenarios) that can be reused to validate the outcomes with the participants in iterative manners. It is also one of the main ideas of research-based design (Mattelmäki, 2006). Such outcomes and work processes cannot be acquired when using digital tools such as Atlas.ti. Three researchers executed the primary analysis. The preliminary analysis of the process outputs was shared with four researchers from the project’s partner universities to validate the analysis and provide a broader perspective. The process’ outcomes of the focus groups led to canvases of classification and concept maps. These were summarised by the emerged themes (see Figure 2a, b and c). Based on the previous outcomes, journey maps and inspiration cards were created for discussion with the participants. From the focus groups, workshops, and interviews, we had over 150 post-it notes. The data was analysed again by bottom-up note comparison between the researchers and categorised by emerging theme activities, context and tools. These provided the basis for scenarios to be used in prototype design. See the research team’s outcomes blog3.

The concept of validity has been defined in various ways (Onwuegbuzie & Johnson, 2006). We followed the description of Teddlie and Tashakkori (2003), who see validity in qualitative research as inference transferability and inference quality. Inference quality is the identification of four evaluation criteria (a) within-design consistency, i.e., consistency of the design of the study and from which the inference emerged; (b) conceptual consistency is the degree to which the inferences are consistent with each other and with the known state of knowledge and theory; (c) interpretive agreement is the consistency of interpretations across people and (d) interpretations of the results and rival explanations are ruled out.

The analysis methods of part b) prototyping

The qualitative and thematic analysis of the part B prototyping was executed by comparing three researchers’ notes and interpretations of the video recordings. For the analysis, we used triangulation in different researchers and data collection. The researchers transcribed the video recordings and digitalised the handwritten notes. After which, they listed the comments on the data. The prototype testing data were discussed, and similarities in the data were grouped in a similar spatial manner as in part A).

Business Finland supported the HUMEX project (Quantifying Human Experience for Increased Intelligence Within Work Teams and in the Customer Interface). The AALTO university’s Ethical Review Board approved the study protocol. All participants signed written informed consent.

Section 4 describes the outcomes of the contextual inquiry and explains the choices made regarding the prototype to be designed with the participants. The findings indicated the need to narrow our hypothesis and simplify the prototype ideas.

Findings from the contextual inquiry (part a)

After analysing the data from the focus groups (topics in focus groups: Facilitating a focus group using video-conferencing; Group collaboration using audio-conference; audio conference and Community building using Instant Messaging) where all the participants (insurance and legal service/company, development, design and marketing and construction sectors ) were involved, the research team created a summary in the form of three themes – the topic description canvases. The themes recapitulate the most critical findings from focus groups’ data (Fig.1). This outcome was a result of three (3) iterations. As a summary, the analysis revealed that people’s awareness of interlocutors’ presence during synchronous communication is a crucial need for successful collaboration, trust-building and shared understanding. These are difficult to establish in digitally mediated communications. Another finding was that the attitudes towards tracking arousal and the proposed interpretations of the state of the participants varied considerably. The arousal tracking was confusing to all except one participant. This one participant had executed festival micro-expression mapping from facial recognition of the crowd to the festival organisers. The others understood the arousal tracking to be data from facial recognition systems, owned or gathered by outside party sensory data. All of the above use algorithmic systems; thus, the hesitancy can be related to data use and accuracy of algorithmic systems and privacy.

The three canvases of the main themes are presented in Figure 2. We will summarise each one of them. We have not separated the outcomes by the workplace sector the participants represent because there was not that much variation between sectors; instead, variation occurred between individuals. Most participants were cautious of the new and emerging technologies. For us research designers, it was interesting to find out if the participants brought something new into the scene. Thus, we first summarise the participants’ views, present some quotations from them and then provide a summary of already known challenges.

The bottom-up themes formulated to consist of a) presence – being able to feel the presence of others, b) trust – being able to build trust with others and c) shared understanding – knowing when shared understanding has been achieved. Below we summarise the findings using the above three themes from the focus groups. As mentioned, we focused on the H1 – topics of the attitudes towards emotion tracking, how the participants see it could be used to enhance their work, current practises and what is not working at work regarding digitally mediated collaboration and communication. The descriptions of the themes start with a summary of outcomes and quotations from the participants, relation to previous research (literature) to see if the participants came up with something not discussed before and what this means for future tools, and what we need to consider.

Presence

Summary

The participants explained presence4 as feeling the presence of the other interlocutor. This feeling may come holistically, or it can be sensed by one dominant sensory channel. Presence is essential in itself and as it enables knowing whether the other interlocutor is engaged in active listening. These formulations were used by the participants of the contextual inquiry (Figure 2left side). 

In this case, the tracking was understood as the system’s default action, and attentiveness was presented based on the automatically tracked data. The participants wished to have control over what would be displayed for other interlocutors to see. Another concern was the system’s amount of interpretation based on the data. For instance, it was mentioned that the system might interpret that one is not attentive and not present incorrectly due to personal differences that vary concerning the average of the measurements that are used by the system. The audio was mentioned as a way to understand presence. 

Participants mentioning the audio/voice-related issues:

“Audio communications tend to be more “clinical” as people tend to make their comments and dialogue in a more condensed way. The conversation can be more straight, built is less rich than in F2F.”

“Tone of voice, I have the image I do get the emotional involvement level view voice as well.”

” But in big conversation it is the official voice. Lecture mode the one is group. Reporting situations are also that kind of non-emotional voice situations. “

“It is something common that in these contexts someone takes over, since these tools do not enable normal signs to show that you disagree, agree or so on, so giving this type of feedback is challenging. Voice is still is the only way to express yourself, even when there is video.”

Other interesting statements from the participants related to tracking o expressions and control:

“In festivals analysis object and people - video-analysis for instance, XYZ festival, what people feel, micro-expressions - analysis of emotions and visible expressions for festival organisers.”

“Feedback through other channels than video or audio – especially in the case of big presentations, would benefit from using other ways of sensing since this would allow keep eye contact.”

“If the data tracked consists in a   voluntary expression of people’s   mood, it is OK” Obtaining/giving feedback that is voluntary (like thumbs up, sending question marks…) – if it not verbal, e.g. thumbs up, down, nodding and so forth would feel comfortable. 

“It’s important to know beforehand if one is being tracked. Tracking could be beneficial as it re-inforce personal connection (presence?), help concentrating, performing active listening.”

  “Might be automated but I have the control and then I could do something, decide how the convey those signs.”

“More control on how these data are conveyed”... “I prefer tracking at group level. “

“We want to have a good level of empathy in communications with customers– it it relates to the attitude of the person who is attending you, it is important that you feel that the other person really wants to understand your case “

There were few suggestions, but one caught our eye, which was about how to code the overall atmosphere: “Colour coding if I am losing people overall feeling of the group.”

Previous Research

In the context of this design research, the concept idea of presence (related to awareness, see Biocca et al. 2003) consists of supporting different types of feedback, which allows for knowing the extent to which people taking part are present in a conversation or joint work mediated by digital tools. The array of tools that could provide this type of feedback changes according to the context, the conversation purpose and the type of presence that is being performed (Rogers & Lea, 2005).

Presence has also been connected to empathy, or the ability to understand the feelings of the other and the possibility to be present (Iacoboni et al., 2005). Recently, empathy has been defined as a skill that can be learned, even if it is still composed of different aspects, such as 1) experience sharing: vicariously sharing targets’ internal states, 2) mentalising: explicitly considering (and perhaps understanding) targets’ states and their sources, and 3) prosocial concern: expressing motivation to improve targets’ experiences (for example, by reducing their suffering) (Zaki & Ochsner, 2012). Empathy can be viewed as part of the more frequently used definition of social presence (Sivunen & Nordbäck, 2015). Sivunen and Nordbäck (2015) defined empathy as composed of three aspects: a) co-presence, such as feeling inclusion or isolation and mutual awareness; b) psychological involvement, referring to mutual attention, empathy and shared understanding; and c) behavioural engagement, referring to behavioural interaction and dependent action. In this regard, social presence can be viewed as a group/team-level construct and observed in interactions between interlocutors.

Recently Grassini and Laumann (2020) executed a systematic literature review and brought up that only Lee (2004) has managed to provide some consistency into the concept of presence. Presence can be separated into three distinct domains: physical, social, and self. Physical refers to the experience of the physical environment and the objects within the environment, social directs to the experience of other entities with social value within the environment, and self-experience describes the experiences the user has of him/herself.

Meaning for Design

From the current research (Grassini & Laumann, 2020), it seems like the participants had reasonable suspicion since, so far, it is not possible to measure the feeling of presence accurately. The effects that the context has on the discussion, the role one has in the discussion, and the subject matter determine the amount of attention required to follow attentively. However, the participants also mentioned that if the data on presence would be acquired from the group activity where the information is anonymous, it might be acceptable (Quan-Haase et al., 2005; Sivunen & Nordbäck, 2015 on favouring group/team-level constructs). Thus, we may say that it can be accepted if it is general tracking and no persons can be detected. The suggestions and recommendations can be controlled by the users (participants). Being in control seems a valuable design feature as it is also often required by the algorithmic systems – having the human in the loop (Leslie & Briggs, 2021).

Shared understanding

Summary

For the participants, shared understanding5 meant arriving at and knowing when there is an agreed understanding of a topic of discussion. The concept aims to provide tools for sharing and receiving information that will help detect whether people have a shared understanding of the topic under study (Figure 2 middle). It was difficult for participants to determine what the system could track to help them understand that there is shared understanding. The participants requested cues for positive feelings and agreement, which they saw as supporting the building of the shared understanding. These feelings were more comfortable to digest than what the system displays about participants’ feelings. Positive emotions are more natural to accept as the system’s interpretation of what to present to other interlocutors, even without controlling what is displayed — some points made by the participants.

“Getting feedback about how well people are following/understanding could help adapt the speech (slow down or modifying the explanation).“

“Human touch” in their communications. It relates to the attitude of the person who is attending you; it is important that you feel that the other person really wants to understand your case.” 

“We want to have a good level of empathy in communications with customers– it it relates to the attitude of the person who is attending you, it is important that you feel that the other person really wants to understand your case“ 

“We want to have a good level of empathy in communications with customers– it it relates to the attitude of the person who is attending you, it is important that you feel that the other person really wants to understand your case“

Human touch” in their communications. It relates to the attitude of the person who is attending you, it is important that you feel that the other person really wants to understand your case”

“Virtual object that you are touching together - shared something.”

“The more people get involved is through virtual objects, something you share with others. “An example of drawing - if members start drawing with me, this is a sign they are really interested and are willing to “buy” services/product. Therefore, making participation physical through a shared object contributes to increasing involvement.”

Figure. 2. The themes as we presented them to participants for validation if we grasped their meaning well enough.

Previous research

Shared understanding is a quintessential feature for successful joint and collaborative work practises, especially in the transition from dialogical practises to collaborative practises executed through shared artefacts (Paavola & Miettinen, 2019; Stahl, 2016). Most of the investigated work tasks were precisely these activities during which the participants were collaboratively and systematically developing shared, concrete artefacts together towards a common aim (see Appendix 1 on the scenarios drafted later in the process). A common ground ensures negotiation related to the shared artefacts (Clark & Brennan, 1991). In the work settings described by the participants, indexical signs were used to indicate tangible objects. These signs referred to actual occurrences and experiences familiar to those communicating to convey a meaning shared through similar experiences. The interlocutors share similar experiences through the common ground and correct and reshape the common ground during the conversation (Arrighi & Ferrario, 2008). Recently common ground supporting shared understanding has been researched within the scope of music interpretations (Schober & Neta, 2016). It means that the participants of the event augment the base for forming a shared understanding. The joint base of, e.g., experiences has also been referred to as ‘anchored discussion’ (Van der Pol, 2007). From the perspective of technologies, these should provide supporting affordances and cues for fluent and organised joint work with shared artefacts and practises (Bayer, Campbell, & Ling, 2015). The workshops’ participants’ thoughts were aligned with previous research exploring challenges for shared understanding, such as interpersonal differences, the absence of shared history and context (Hinds & Mortensen, 2005) and a lack of information related to the team members and tool ecosystems used (Bayer et al., 2015).

Meaning for design

The participants generally mentioned sharing objects or doing things together as necessary. Thus, allowing collaboration focussed collaboration might support shared understanding. The other worries that arose were following the others, understanding if they are on the same page, or engaged, possibilities to show empathy. For design, it would mean enhanced means to collaborate without attention distracters. We could imagine that using other senses to detect the other participants' eagerness and ability to follow might provide some solutions.

Trust

Summary

According to the workshop participants, trust6 relates to feeling comfortable and safe and is associated with a relaxed atmosphere where it is possible to fail or express oneself poorly, such as forgetting words or using incorrect terms and incomplete sentences. We heard ideas such as: “Critical to have is trust – there is no trust the system conveying in the right manner.”

“Like when seeing people’s faces - problem in video-conf tools like skype is that people’s face icons are too small and it’s difficult to get that information) – depending on trust level, useful to get participants’ feedback (from 1 to 10) about how was their day – make the conversation more human and give useful feedback about the state (mood, involvement) of the person “

“Trust is also built by the company culture or let’s say the company culture has strong effect on how trust is built. It is not so much about horizontal or vertical company structure. The company needs to assists, acceptance agreement, raw data doesn’t help – needs to be aggregated.”

Trust is not built by the coffee mate app (nor by giving “hugs”) but by actions. “

The concept emphasises creating bonds through shared experiences (Figure 2 right side). For two companies (marketing and design & development – fusion company), the aim was to build and maintain a more profound work culture of respect, appreciation and trust with a homely atmosphere. The participants expressed the need to maintain the homely feeling even when the companies grow by promoting interactions, such as organising meetings between different offices from different countries.

For example, a heartbeat without a system interpreting what the heartbeat means was more acceptable than when the system would present an interpretation of what an increase in a heartbeat might mean (below as one of the prototypes developed further and see Wikström et al., 2021).

Previous Research

A key aspect of trust-building relies on the feeling that one is respected and supported (Boon & Holmes, 1991). Personal connections might help develop trust, but they are not sufficient (Preece, 2004). Kuo and Yu (2009) divided trust into three categories. The first is calculus-based trust in distant teamwork. Calculus-based trust emerges based on a member’s assessment of the outcomes and costs of maintaining a group relationship. When a team continues to work productively, calculus-based trust transforms into knowledge-based trust (knowledge of others’ competencies allowing the members to make predictions on each other’s behaviours). Ultimately, identity-based trust emerges. It is grounded in the members’ mutual identity and the willingness to support each other (Lewicki, McAllister, & Bies, 1998; Rousseau, Sitkin, Burt, & Camerer, 1998). Identity-based trust develops after the teams have worked together effectively for extended periods or at the end of the project. Järvenpää, Shaw, and Staples (2004) and McKnight Choudhury and Kacmar (2002) found that identity-based trust is dependent on the members’ shared values and norms of obligations, and it allows members to support others.

Meaning for Design

For the current investigation, identity-based trust is attractive because the companies involved were attempting to build and maintain a particular work culture. The considerations on the trustfulness of technological actions come close to how the participants defined their suspicions as there is some risk of mistakes technology may make. However, although the participants had these concerns, they were optimistic that the new technologies would help people be more empathic and supportive towards each other in the future.

To answer the H1, there is a need to use algorithmic systems on generated emotional and behavioural tools to enhance, support and scaffold the digitally mediated collaborative work; we can answer yes. There is a need to use algorithms but not necessarily for suggesting, recommending or providing interpretations of emotions or behaviours. Subtle ways of positive emotions (such as agreement, following, empathy, and understanding of the topic under discussion) were seen to bring positive outcomes. In addition, voluntary feedback presentation – the need to control what is presented and how was another topic of discussion. Tentatively, we could summarise that small attempts towards new technologies were supported, and despite the concerns, there was optimism about the future.

Journey maps, inspiration cards and next steps

After reviewing these three themes in the light of previous research and discussing them with the workshop participants, the journey maps were developed. The journey maps helped obtain a better understanding of the interactions between the actors, tools and the ecosystem where the communications and collaborations occurred. The themes and the journey maps: Community building with instant messaging, Showing engagement (presence) in an online focus group using video-conferencing (audio, no video), Supporting shared understanding among the group using online video-conferencing (audio) (see Figure 3 right for an example) provided relevant insights that informed the creation of scenarios: Facilitating a focus group using video-conferencing, Group discussion using audio-conference and Community building using instant messaging (see Appendix 1) and inspiration cards (see Figure 3 left).

The scenarios presented different experiences with digital tools connected to community building using instant messaging, group discussions using audio-conferencing and facilitating a focus group using video-conferencing with shared artefacts. The researcher presented the scenario at the beginning of the workshop to introduce the theme and provide a concrete example similar to the participants' experiences with CMC and collaboration in the workplace regarding the challenges associated with specific tools. The feedback collected from the interviews and workshops using the inspiration cards and scenarios elucidated that the notion of presence was felt to be the most central aspect of the study. The workshops and interviews underlined again the difficulty of showing negative emotions or the care that these should be expressed constructively:

“Negative feelings can be expressed alone in virtual situations and you can still" look" professional.”

“Constructive fear, frustration, anger, personal antipathy - sometimes yes good to show,

If there is trust, it works, but in the customer meeting - sometimes I even reflect.”

“Presenting individual feelings … No. 70% frustrated - but as group level you get the feelings”

“Shared a thing all of the group feels it - atmosphere.”

“Constructive way to express is needed, group level, maybe from tone of voice and posture, positive things spread, a negative spiral can be spread by one person.”

“Anonymous positive feedback to colleagues creates the bonus system.”

2Perhaps this would not be a good idea if everyone gets same emotional info, possibility to fake, plus loss of control because one ends following the system instructions.”

The participants mentioned that when feelings and emotions, such as agreement and disagreement, are presented in a manner that simulates a face-to-face situation as closely as possible, it is ineffective. These simulated situations require too much concentration in a digitally mediated context to be fruitful. Therefore, for efficiency during work tasks, it is essential to perceive agreement and attention easily.

“Technical quality seemless quality”

“Challenge in remote communications: the more people join the communication, the more info (audio, audiovisual) is coming in and that's difficult to handle from the technical point of view.”

“This type of video conferencing without the video doesn't add anything and it is just like a phone call. On top of that chat is used for common discussion or notes. In a collaborative session like this someone is sharing their screen (who is leading the meeting) and the rest look at that, not faces. Its is easier to see, show screen then explain verbally. “

“In F2F it's easier to find out that the other person is lost, and thus he can adjust (move to a plan B), whereas in remote communication systems noticing is not that easy that the other person is lost.”

“Nodding is helpful in conversations, but it is not; it is used if it has to be done on purpose (both considered emoticons in ‘skype’ are useful, but somehow they are not that used or might be awkward to use if there is not much confidence.”

“Communication over a shared object happens almost all the time – it keeps the communication focused.”

In addition, the display should be straightforward, so it does not disturb actual work. These outcomes directed us to redefine the hypothesis into a narrower scope, and we had to dismantle complex tools ideas for this study to allow appropriation (Dourish, 2003).

Based on the journey maps and scenarios, various prototypes were created. In the HUMEX project's seminar, we presented prototypes created by students. There were two prototypes on voice, communication on shared work support – Nodders and customer service. Twelve participants (unanimously) selected the heartbeat customer service and the Nodders to be continued (see the finalised presentation of the prototypes in Appendix 2). The rest of the prototypes were completed as far as the student teams needed for their grade. The seminar allowed participants to try out the prototypes to understand them better. The student developers and designers were present to answer questions. The heartbeat visualisation for customer service development can be read from Wikström et al. (2021). Both selected prototypes allowed a step-to-step development towards different usage of technologies.

The Nodders prototype explores the challenges associated with the notion of presence. It aims to improve collaboration by providing non-verbal feedback regarding head gestures as an addition to the audio or video communication channel. During the design process, another factor was considered – a tool should evolve over time and with use (Arias, Eden, Fischer, & Gorman 2000; Wulf et al. 2015). The Nodders prototype is meant to be used in an audio or video conference.

Currently, the prototype consists of sensors attached to a headband, which reacts to the nodding7 of the wearer (see Figure 4a and b). The action is rendered close to real-time and is displayed as an animated nodding or shaking of the smiley on a computer screen visible to the participants on the conference call. The prototype design is minimal and uncomplicated to provide the interlocutors with feedback that is easy to process and perceived with peripheral attention (Carroll, Rosson, Farooq, & Xiao, 2009). The literature does not often mention this, but simplicity in collaborative technologies is vital for success. For instance, Nardi and Miller (1991) conducted a study that showed that effortless technologies designed as collaboration tools have proven to be most effective in encouraging collaborative work. These technologies succeed because they allow a smoother evolvement with the use (Arias et al., 2000). The decision to reduce possible sensor-based features was difficult because many ideas related to tracking arousal that could have been designed and developed were abandoned. We dropped out the idea of gaze following (see Pöysä-Tarhonen, Awwal, Häkkinen, & Otieno, 2020) and facial or micro-expression detection (Zhao & Li, 2019) because of the need for more technology to be added into the participants' use. In addition, to avoid such data collection from the participants, they could not control. The Nodders use algorithms to learn the nodding of the person wearing the sensor. Because people nod in different manners, some nod heavily and others lightly, but the participants still control the nodding. Thus, the use of the algorithmic system is minimal, and it is not providing suggestions or recommendations. We could say that it tries to interpret the nodding of a person.

To date, four prototype testing with workers were performed to attain iterative and frequent feedback that informs further development cycles. The first two prototype tests were carried out with simulated tasks. The second two with actual tasks shared artefacts and workers from two companies that participated in the previous focus groups. Next, the findings are described.

The nodders prototype testing and data collection

Analysis of the prototype testing

During the first observations, it was difficult to recognise when the workers and the students noticed the smiley. The first impression was that they ignored it; however, during the interviews, the workers and the student mentioned that they were glancing at the smiley when it nodded. They also expressed that they saw the smiley in their peripheral vision because the movement was detectable as the smiley was otherwise still (Figure 4). For instance, participants stated: “Usually, concentration on another’s presence takes effort. This takes it away from the joint work at hand. Now, I just saw it from the corner of my eye, or I saw it for a moment. That was enough because it only nods or doesn’t. I do not need to concentrate on the expression.”

Similar comments were expressed after the prototype testing with workers from companies. In addition, when observing the second testing, it was easier to notice when the workers reacted to the smiley. The ability to observe better could be due to the researchers learning to pay attention to particular reactions (e.g., small head movements and pauses7) after the first two prototype tests.

References
  1. Ackerman, M. S. (2000). The intellectual challenge of CSCW: The gap between social requirements and technical feasibility. In J. M. Carroll (Ed.), Human Computer Interaction in the New Millennium, (pp. 303–324). New York: ACM Press.
  2. Anderson, J. F. Beard, F. K., & Walther, J. B. (2010). Turn-taking and the local management of conversation in a highly simultaneous computer-mediated communication system, Language@ Internet. 7(7).
  3. Arias, E., Eden, H., Fischer, G., Gorman, A., & Scharff, E. (2000). Transcending the individual human mind—creating shared understanding through collaborative design. ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction (TOCHI), 7(1), 84–113. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1145/344949.345015
  4. Arrighi, C., & Ferrario, R. (2008). Abductive Reasoning, Interpretation and Collaborative Processes. Foundations of Science, 13, 75–87. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10699-007-9117-4
  5. Bakker, D., Kazantzis, N., Rickwood, D., & Rickard, N. (2016). Mental Health Smartphone Apps: Review and Evidence-Based Recommendations for Future Developments. JMIR mental health. 3(1), e7. https://doi.org/10.2196/mental.4984. DOI: https://doi.org/10.2196/mental.4984
  6. Balters, S., Mayseless, N., Hawthorne, G., & Reiss, A. L. (2021). The Neuroscience of Team Cooperation Versus Team Collaboration. Design Thinking Research: Interrogating the Doing, 203. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-62037-0_9
  7. Bauters, M., Purma, J., & Leinonen, T. (2014). In-time on-place learning. In 10th International Conference on Mobile Learning 2014, (pp. 256–260). IADIS Press.
  8. Bauters, M. (2017). Experience in Design and Learning Approaches–Enhancing the Framework for Experience. Revista Română de Comunicare şi Relaţii Publice, 19(1), 51–68. DOI: https://doi.org/10.21018/rjcpr.2017.1.230
  9. Bayer, J. B., Campbell, S. W., & Ling, R. (2015). Connection Cues: Activating the Norms and Habits of Social Connectedness. Communication Theory, 26(2), 128–149. https://doi.org/10.1111/comt.12090 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/comt.12090
  10. Bhattacharjee, T., Datta, S., Das, D., Choudhury, A. D., Pal, A., & Ghosh, P. K. (2018). A Heart Rate Driven Kalman Filter for Continuous Arousal Trend Monitoring. In 2018 40th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society, (pp. 3572–3577). (EMBC), IEEE DOI: https://doi.org/10.1109/EMBC.2018.8513073
  11. Biocca, F., Harms, C., & Burgoon, J. K. (2003). Toward a more robust theory and measure of social presence: Review and suggested criteria. Presence: Teleoperators & virtual environments. 12(5), 456–480. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/105474603322761270
  12. Boon, S. D., & Holmes, J. G. (1991). The dynamics of interpersonal trust: Resolving uncertainty in the face of risk. In R. A. Hinde & J. Groebel (Eds.), Cooperation and Prosocial Behavior, (pp. 190–211). Oxford, K: Basil Blackwell.
  13. Cameron, A. F., & Webster, J. (2005). Unintended consequences of emerging communication technologies: Instant messaging in the workplace. Computers in Human behavior. 21(1), 85–103. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2003.12.001
  14. Carroll, J. M., Rosson, M. B., Farooq, U., & Xiao, L. (2009). Beyond being aware. Information and Organization. 19, 162–185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infoandorg.2009.04.004 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infoandorg.2009.04.004
  15. Clark, H. H., & Brennan, S. E. (1991). Grounding in communication. In L. B. Resnick, J. M. Levine, and S. D. Teasley (Eds.), Perspectives on socially shared cognition, (pp. 127–149). Washington, DC: APA Books. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/10096-006
  16. Dourish, P., & Bellotti, V. (1992). Awareness and coordination in shared workspaces. In Proceedings of the Conference on Computer-Supported Cooperative Work, Toronto. 107–114. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1145/143457.143468
  17. Dourish, P. (2003). The appropriation of interactive technologies: Some lessons from placeless documents. Computer Supported Cooperative Work. 12(4), 465–490. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026149119426
  18. Feenberg, A. (2010). Between reason and experience: Essays in technology and modernity. MIT Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/8221.001.0001
  19. Fiore, S. M., & Wiltshire, T. J. (2016). Technology as teammate: Examining the role of external cognition in support of team cognitive processes. Frontiers in psychology. 7, 1531. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01531. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01531
  20. Garro-Abarca, V., Palos-Sanchez, P. & Aguayo-Camacho, M. (2021). Virtual Teams in Times of Pandemic: Factors That Influence Performance. Front. Psychol. 12, 624–637. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.624637 DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.624637
  21. Grassini, S., & Laumann, K. (2020). Questionnaire measures and physiological correlates of presence: A systematic review. Frontiers in Psychology. 11, 349. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00349
  22. Gravina, R., Parastoo, A., Ghasemzadeh, H., & Fortino, G. (2017). Multi-sensor fusion in body sensor networks: State-of-the-art and research challenges. Information Fusion. 35, 68–80. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.inffus.2016.09.005
  23. Gross, T. (2013). Supporting effortless coordination: 25 years of awareness research. Computer Supported Cooperative Work (CSCW). 22 (4–6), 425–474. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10606-013-9190-x
  24. Grudin, J. (1994). Groupware and social dynamics: Eight challenges for developers. Communications of the ACM. 37(1), 92–105. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1145/175222.175230
  25. Gutwin, C., & Greenberg, S. (1998). Design for individuals, design for groups: tradeoffs between power and workspace awareness, in Proceedings of CSCW’98, (pp. 207–216). Seattle, WA). DOI: https://doi.org/10.1145/289444.289495
  26. Hannola, L., Richter, A., Richter, S., & Stocker, A. (2018). Empowering production workers with digitally facilitated knowledge processes–a conceptual framework. International Journal of Production Research. 56(14), 4729-4743. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/00207543.2018.1445877
  27. Hinds, P. J., & Mortensen, M. (2005). Understanding conflict in geographically distributed teams: The moderating effects of shared identity, shared context, and spontaneous communication. Organization Science. 16(3), 290–307. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1287/orsc.1050.0122
  28. Hontvedt, M., & Øvergård, K. I. (2019). Simulations at Work—a Framework for Configuring Simulation Fidelity with Training Objectives. Computer Supported Cooperative Work (CSCW), 1–29. https://doi.org/1-29.10.1007/s10606-019-09367-8 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10606-019-09367-8
  29. Iacoboni, M., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one's own mirror neuron system. PLoS biology. 3(3), e79. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030079
  30. Ingusci, E., Signore, F., Giancaspro, M. L., Manuti, A., Molino, M., Russo, V., Zito, M. & Cortese, C. G. (2021). Workload, Techno Overload, and Behavioral Stress During COVID-19 Emergency: The Role of Job Crafting in Remote Workers. Front. Psychol. 12:655148. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.655148 DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.655148
  31. Järvenpää, S. L., Shaw, T.R., & Staples, D. S. (2004). Toward Contextualized Theories of Trust: The Role of Trust in Global Virtual Teams. Information Systems Research, INFORMS. 15(3), 250–267, from http://www.jstor.org/stable/23015796. Accessed 25 July 2019. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1287/isre.1040.0028
  32. Kiesler, S., Siegel, J., & McGuire, T. W. (1984). Social psychological aspects of computer-mediated communication. American psychologist. 39(10), 1123. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.39.10.1123
  33. Koskinen, I., Zimmerman, J., Binder, T. Redstrom, J. & Wensveen, S. (2013). Design Research Through Practice: From the Lab, Field, and Showroom. IEEE Transactions on Professional Communication. 56(3), 262–263. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1109/TPC.2013.2274109
  34. Kruger, J., Epley, N., Parker, J., & Ng, Z.-W. (2005). Egocentrism over e-mail: Can we communicate as well as we think?. Journal of personality and social psychology. 89(6), 925. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.925
  35. Kuo, F.-y., & Yu, C.-p. (2009). An exploratory study of trust dynamics in work-oriented virtual teams. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication. 14(4), 823–854. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2009.01472.x
  36. Mackay, W. (2000). Responding to cognitive overload: Co-adaptation between users and technology. Intellectica. 30(1), 177–193. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3406/intel.2000.1597
  37. Latour B. (1999). On Recalling Ant. The Sociological Review. 47(1_suppl), 15–25. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-954X.1999.tb03480.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-954X.1999.tb03480.x
  38. Lee, K. M. (2004). Presence, explicated. Commun. Theory. 14, 27–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2885.2004.tb00302.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2885.2004.tb00302.x
  39. Leslie, D., & Briggs, M. (2021). Explaining decisions made with AI: A workbook (Use case 1: AI-assisted recruitment tool). Available at SSRN 3808512. https://ssrn.com/abstract=3808512 or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3808512 DOI: https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3808512
  40. Lewicki, R. J., McAllister, D. J., & Bies, R. J. (1998). Trust and distrust: New relationships and realities. Academy of Management Review. 23(3), 438–458. DOI: https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1998.926620
  41. Mattelmäki, T. (2006). Design probes. Aalto University. Publication Series of the University of Art and Design Helsinki A, 69/2006.
  42. McKnight, D. H., Choudhury, & V., Kacmar, C. (2002). Developing and validating trust measures for e-commerce: An integrative typology. Information Systems Research. 13(3), 334–359. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1287/isre.13.3.334.81
  43. Mueller, F., Maes, P., & Grudin, J. (2019). Human-Computer Integration (Dagstuhl Seminar 18322). Schloss Dagstuhl-Leibniz-Zentrum fuer Informatik. https://doi.org/10.4230/DagRep.8.8.18
  44. Nardi, B. A., & Miller, J. R. (1991). Twinkling lights and nested loops: Distributed problem solving and spreadsheet development. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies. 34(2), 161–184. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/0020-7373(91)90040-E
  45. Noble, S. U., & Roberts, S. T. (2016). Through Google-colored glass (es): Design, emotion, class, and wearables as commodity and control. Emotions, Technology, and Design (Academic Press. ) 187–212. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-801872-9.00009-0
  46. Onwuegbuzie, A. J., & Johnson, R. B. (2006). The validity issue in mixed research. Research in the Schools. 13(1), 48–63.
  47. Paavola, S., & Miettinen, R. (2019). Dynamics of Design Collaboration: BIM Models as Intermediary Digital Objects. Comput Supported Coop Work. 28, 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10606-018-9306-4. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10606-018-9306-4
  48. Peake, J. M., Kerr, G., & Sullivan, J. P. (2018). A critical review of consumer wearables, mobile applications, and equipment for providing biofeedback, monitoring stress, and sleep in physically active populations. Front. Physiol. 28(9), 743. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2018.00743. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2018.00743
  49. Pejoska, J., Bauters, M., Purma, J., & Leinonen, T. (2016). Social augmented reality: Enhancing context‐dependent communication and informal learning at work. British Journal of Educational Technology. 47(3), 474–483 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/bjet.12442
  50. Pirinen, A. (2016). The barriers and enablers of co-design for services. International Journal of Design, 10(3), 27–42.
  51. Preece, J. (2004). Etiquette, empathy and trust in communities of practice: Stepping-stones to social capital. JUCS. 10(3), 294–302.
  52. Pöysä-Tarhonen, J., Awwal, N., Häkkinen, P., & Otieno, S. (2020). From monitoring to sharing of attention in dyadic interaction : The affordances of gaze data to better understand social aspects of remote collaborative problem solving. In H.-J. So, M. M. Rodrigo, J. Mason, & A. Mitrovic (Eds.), ICCE 2020 : Proceedings of the 28th International Conference on Computers in Education, Volume I (pp. 109-118). Asia-Pacific Society for Computers in Education, from https://apsce.net/upfile/icce2020/ICCE%202020%20Proceedings%20-%20Volume%20I%20v4.pdf
  53. Quan-Haase, A., Cothrel, J., & Wellman, B. (2005). Instant Messaging for Collaboration: A Case Study of a High-Tech Firm. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication. 10(4) JCMC10413. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2005.tb00276.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2005.tb00276.x
  54. Ramstead, M. JD., Veissière, S. PL. & Kirmayer, L. J. (2016). Cultural affordances: Scaffolding local worlds through shared intentionality and regimes of attention. Frontiers in psychology. 7, 1090. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01090
  55. Rogers, P., & Lea, M. (2005). Social presence in distributed group environments: The role of socialidentity. Behaviour & Information Technology. 24(2), 151–158. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/01449290410001723472
  56. Rousseau, D. M., Sitkin, S. B., Burt, R. S., & Camerer, C. (1998). Not so different after all: A cross-disciplinary view of trust. Academy Management Review. 23(3), 393–404. DOI: https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1998.926617
  57. Salminen, M., Järvelä, S., Ruonala, A., Timonen, J., Mannermaa, K., Ravaja, N., et al. (2018). Bio- adaptive social VR to evoke affective interdependence – DYNECOM. In 23rd international conference on intelligent user interfaces, (pp. 73–77). DOI: https://doi.org/10.1145/3172944.3172991
  58. Sanders, E. B-N. (2003). From user-centered to participatory design approaches, in Design and the social sciences, CRC Press. 18–25. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1201/9780203301302-8
  59. Schmidt, K. & Bannon, L. J. (1992). Taking CSCW seriously: Supporting articulation work, Computer Supported Cooperative Work (CSCW): An International Journal. 1(1–2), 7–40. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00752449
  60. Schober, M. F., & Spiro, N. (2016). Listeners' and performers' shared understanding of jazz improvisations. Frontiers in psychology, 7, 1629. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01629
  61. Sivunen, A., & Nordbäck, E. (2015). Social Presence as a Multi-Dimensional Group Construct in 3D Virtual Environments. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication. 20(1), 19–36. doi:10.1111/jcc4.12090. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/jcc4.12090
  62. Stahl, G. (2016). From intersubjectivity to group cognition. Computer Supported Cooperative Work (CSCW). 25(4–5), 355–384. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10606-016-9243-z. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10606-016-9243-z
  63. Stewart, D. W., & Shamdasani, P. N. (2014). Focus groups: Theory and practice. 20. Sage publications.
  64. Teddlie, C., & Tashakkori, A. (2003). Major Issue and controversies in the use of mixed methods in the social and behavioural sciences. In C. Tashakkori & A. Teddlie (Eds.), A Handbook of mixed methods in social and behavioural research, (pp. 3–50.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage).
  65. Trenerry, B., Chng, S., Wang, Y., Suhaila, Z. S., Lim, S. S., Lu HY & Oh, P. H. (2021). Preparing Workplaces for Digital Transformation: An Integrative Review and Framework of Multi-Level Factors. Front. Psychol. 12, 620766. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.620766 DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.620766
  66. Trigg, R. H., & Bødker, S. (1994). From implementation to design: Tailoring and the emergence of systematization in CSCW. In Proceedings of the ACM Conference on Computer-Supported Cooperative Work, CSCW. (pp. 45–54). Chapel Hill: ACM Press, '94. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1145/192844.192869
  67. Törpel, B., Pipek, V., & Rittenbruch, M. (2003). Creating heterogeneity: Evolving use of groupware in a network of freelancers. Computer Supported Cooperative Work. 12(4), 381– 409. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026192900770
  68. Van der Pol, J. (2007). Facilitating online learning conversations. Exploring tool affordances in higher education. Dissertation. IVLOS Series, Utrecht.
  69. Wikström, V., Makkonen, T., & Saarikivi, K. (2017). SynKin: A Game for Intentionally Synchronizing Biosignals. In Proceedings of the 2017 CHI Conference Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI EA ’17), (pp. 3005–3011). ACM, New York, NY, USA, https://doi.org/10.1145/3027063.3053195. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1145/3027063.3053195
  70. Wikström, V., Falcon, M., Martikainen, S., Pejoska, J.,Durall, E. Bauters, M., & Saarikivi, S. (2021). Heart Rate Sharing at the Workplace. Multimodal Technologies and Interaction 5(10), 60. https://doi.org/10.3390/mti5100060. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3390/mti5100060
  71. Wulf, V., Müller, C., Pipek, V., Randall, D., Rohde, M., & Stevens, G. (2015). Practice-based computing: Empirically grounded conceptualizations derived from design case studies. In Designing socially embedded technologies in the real-world, (pp. 111–150.) Springer, London. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4471-6720-4_7
  72. Zaki, J., & Ochsner, K. N. (2012). The neuroscience of empathy: progress, pitfalls and promise. Nature Neuroscience. 15(5), 675. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3085
  73. Zhao, X., Lampe, C., & Ellison, N. B. (2016). The social media ecology: User perceptions, strategies and challenges. In Proceedings of the 2016 CHI conference on human factors in computing systems, (pp. 89–100 ). ACM. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1145/2858036.2858333
  74. Zhao, G., & Li, X. (2019). Automatic micro-expression analysis: open challenges. Frontiers in psychology, 10, 1833. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01833

How to Cite

Bauters, M., Pejoska, J., Durall, E., Saarikivi, K., Wikström, V., Falcon, M., & Martikainen, S. (2021). Are you there? Presence in collaborative distance work. Human Technology, 17(3), 261–293. https://doi.org/10.14254/1795-6889.2021.17-3.5